0


CHAPTER TWO
2.0 REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE
2.1    Introduction
This chapter focuses on earlier studies carried out on child’s social and language development process. The available related literature is being reviewed under the following sub-headings:
v    Overview of language
v    Language Content
v    The Communication Environment
v    Developmental stages of Chilgren
v    Theories of language acquisition
v    Behaviorism
v    Innativism
v    Vygoitsky’s theory of language
v    Review of Empirical studies on Language Acquisition

2.1 Overview of Language
 Language has been hailed as the hallmark of humanity, the ability that separates humans from animals (Berko-Gleason, 1997). As humans in society, we use our language ability continuously to embrace ideas, share our feelings, comment on the world, and understand each other’s minds. Language can be defined as an organized system of arbitrary signals and rule-governed structures that are used as a means for communication. Language occurs both receptively and expressively through reading, listening, writing, and speaking. In order to become fully functioning members of school and society, children must learn the elements, the rules, the structure, and the conventions of this system. Our working definition of language encompasses five structural components: phonology, semantics, syntax, morphology, and pragmatics. Phonology refers to the sounds and the intonation patterns that are associated with spoken language. Semantics encompasses the words of a language and the meanings associated with those words. Syntax describes the grammatical rules of a language—how words combine into phrases and sentences. Morphology refers to the rules that govern the use of morphemes. Morphemes are the smallest units of meaning in a language, including the prefixes and suffixes that mark syntactic and semantic information, such as number (i.e., plurals), gender, and tense (i.e., past, present, or future). Finally, pragmatics is how we adjust our speech to our audience and use language toward the goal of communication. To better address typical and atypical language development as well as strategies of prevention and intervention, the five structural components of language may be simplified into three essential aspects of communication: content, form, and use (Bloom & Lahey, 1978). Content refers to the semantics of language—the concepts and ideas that are encoded in words. Form is the way in which meaning is represented, including speech, sign language, and writing. In the context of spoken language, form encompasses phonology, morphology, and syntax.
Finally, use refers to the function of language in context. Although each of these aspects of language can be identified separately, they are inherently interconnected elements in communication (Bloom & Lahey). Language problems may arise when there is a disruption within any one component of the model or in their integration.
2.3 Language Content
Relations between words and their referents are arbitrary and symbolic. Words themselves do not lend the language learner any clues to the identity of what is being labeled. Thus, learning the meaning of words involves learning how one’s own language community labels content in the world. This is not an easy task. Consider, for example, seeing a rabbit hopping by and hearing the word rabbit. How does one know whether the word applies to the whole rabbit or to its fur? To its twitching whiskers or to its hopping? Research suggests that even very young children are guided in this initial word-to-referent mapping by a default set of assumptions or predispositions (see Woodward & Markman, 1998, for a review of these principles). Hypotheses based on these assumptions are then supplemented by input and feedback from mature speakers, allowing children to test and revise their label to referent mappings in order to conform to those of their speech community.
The semantic achievement of the production of the first word typically occurs around the child’s first birthday; however, it may appear as early as 8 months or as late as 16 months without indicating serious concern. By this time, children have already been exposed to a great deal of language and possess a receptive vocabulary of about 50 words (Fenson et al., 1994). Once vocabulary learning begins, progress is slow and measured. However, by 18 months of age productive vocabularies typically expand to about 50 words. Great individual differences appear during this period. For example, although at 16 months the average number of words a baby can say is 40, the top 10% of 16-month-olds can say 180 words, and the bottom 10% can say fewer than 10, and in some cases none at all (Fenson et al., 1994). This variation among children is completely normal and should be expected. During the toddler, preschool, and school years, children continue to acquire a varied lexicon. Semantic development expands from the concrete nouns of infancy to complex, abstract, and relational concepts, such as words for actions, emotions, and colors; and deictic terms, such as I, you, this, and that. Children also make connections among the words in their vocabulary, building a complex network of interrelated words and concepts.
Semantic development does not end in childhood. Even into adulthood we continue to add new words to our lexicon (e.g., blog and latte), and fine-tune the content of our communication as we increase our knowledge and experiences (see Pan, 2001, for a review of semantic development).





2.4  The communication environment
Although there is a clear case for the role played by social disadvantage in predicting language skills in young children, the studies often make it difficult to disentangle precisely what it is about children’s social backgrounds that are important for child development. For this reason we are developing, in this present study, a model of risk which is specifically tied to the child’s communication environment. Specifically we are interested in what parents do to promote the communication skills of their child, what they feel in terms of the support they receive and what they have in terms of the materials at their disposal to facilitate the child’s communication. Knowing, for example, that a family lives in council rented accommodation will give some indication of material poverty but knowing the specific resources at the disposal to the parent and child and what they do with those resources is likely to be much more informative about the process by which the child’s communication skills are encouraged and ultimately the child’s preparedness for school (Locke et al, 2002). Thus we hypothesise that the communication environment comprises:
                        What parents do: aspects of the mother’s activity and interaction with the child;
                        What parents feel: specifically, the mother’s feelings, attitudes and sense of wellbeing;
                        What parents have: resources which are available and underpin activity with the child.
2.4.1 What parents do:
Recent analyses have tended to be circumspect about the precise characteristics of environments which do and which do not promote language development. Probably the most influential single study in the field has been Hart and Risley’s study (1995) of the verbal input received by children from different social groups. The results very clearly indicate that the children from so called “welfare mothers” consistently received less verbal input than children from blue collar or professional parents. Recent argument has developed this further, using larger representative samples as their source. For example Pan and colleagues (2005) have suggested that it is not the amount that a mother talks to her child that predicts how their language will subsequently develop but it is the number of word types used by the mother and the language and literacy levels of the mother. This suggests that it is something about the nature of those parental skills which interacts with the communication needs of the child, specifically their sensitivity to the child (Hoff-Ginsberg, 1995). This is supported from an analysis of the 10% “Children in Focus” sample from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort. Boyle and colleagues (Boyle et al submitted) suggested that different cognitive styles, measured from clips of videoed parent/child interaction at one year of age, are associated with parenting
behaviour and appear to be associated with different aspects of communication development two years later.
Interestingly there is a tension developing in the literature over when parental input plays the most significant role. While many argue that the environment plays a role from the word go and thus that intervention should be targeted at the earliest possible age, a recent strand of research suggests that the influences of the parenting environment increases as the role of “biological” factors decreases. For example, reports from Australia’s ELVS study suggest that the variance accounted for by environmental factors increases from less than 10% of the variance in expressive and receptive language skills at two years to 20% by four years (Reilly et al, 2007& 2010). In short, while the social circumstances in which the child is raised in the early years are clearly linked to early language development, it remains unclear which specific aspects of the child’s early experiences are most important and which are most likely to be modifiable at which time point. Indeed it is likely that the higher level, more distal social risk factors (such as maternal education, income or social deprivation generally) will be less amenable to change than the more proximal factors that affect the day to day interactions of the child with their parents and others in their immediate environment.


2.4.2 What parents feel:
The child’s mother or primary carer, is a crucial component of a child’s communication environment and as outlined above, the mother has been the focus of much research investigating how she interacts with her child in order to facilitate language development (Hart & Risley, 1995). It is the mother’s communicative behaviour that has dominated this research (Hart & Risley, 1995; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998) as opposed to considering what factors may underlie this behaviour. The mother’s communicative behaviour is very dependent on her capacity to understand the need to interact with her child, her motivation to engage in this process and the quality of the communication environment that she is able to provide. Furthering our understanding of the mother’s attachment to her child can inform us of how mothers can be supported to provide a facilitative communication environment for her child.
Although research in this area is under-developed, there are three factors reported in the literature that are taken to define a mother’s capacity. These are 1) the age and experience of the mother; 2) her mental well being and 3) her attachment to the child. Age and experience has been studied by comparing the language abilities of children born to teenage mothers to children of older mothers. Preliminary research suggests that the young children of teenage mothers have poorer language abilities compared to the children of older mothers (Keown et al, 2001). Teenage mothers are reported to provide a communicative environment that has a negative impact on their child’s language development by using less complex and varied 
language and being more directive and less facilitative in their actions than older mothers (Keown et al, 2001; Oxford & Spieker, 2006). The lack of adequate measures to control for social background and the small sample sizes compound the findings from these studies. Nevertheless there is an important question to consider of whether the communicative behaviour of mothers is dependent on their age and experience or even their own language and literacy abilities (as outlined earlier), and their communicative competence which may well not be fully developed in their teenage years.
The impact of maternal depression on children’s general development is well documented and is known to be a significant risk factor for poor attachment in the first few years of a child’s life. Maternal depression certainly limits the mother’s capacity to care for her child, to stimulate her general development and to form a robust attachment. In contrast, much less is known about the mechanics by which maternal depression impacts specifically on a young child’s language development and the role of the attachment process in this. One proposal is that maternal depression impacts negatively on the attachment process which then leads to a reduction in the amount of shared time a mother will interact with her child and ultimately resulting in fewer opportunities for a child to develop their language skills (Paulson et al, 2009). However, the literature is inconclusive about the impact of parental depression. On the one hand Pan et al (2005) found that maternal depression had a negative impact on the child’s rate of vocabulary growth between the ages of one and three years in children of low income families. On the other, Paulson et al (2009) found an association between both maternal and paternal depression and the amount of parent-to-child reading that took place, but only the father’s depression impacted on child’s language development (expressive vocabulary) at 24 months.
Some of the literature around child neglect and language development may be of relevance here, although it is acknowledged that this literature is very specific to this group of children. The few studies in this area to date suggest that severe parental neglect has more of an impact on children’s language development than physical abuse (Culp et al, 1991; Allen & Oliver, 1982; Eigsti & Cicchetti, 2004). Explanations for this finding are weak but propose that children who are neglected are more likely to stay in the home where language stimulation is poor whereas children who are physically abused are usually removed from the parental home into a more verbally stimulating environment.
`The relevance of these findings is in trying to understand whether the impoverished language of children who are severely neglected is due to specific factors related to the impoverished communication environment or the more cumulative effects of social risk. Some insights have been offered into this by a recent study that has identified specific risk factors rather than cumulative effects of social risk. These specific risk factors are 1) maternal depression; 2) the mother’s own history of neglect and/or abuse and 3) the mother’s poor attachment to the child (Sylvestre & Merette, 2010).
Therefore, there is some evidence to show that the mother’s capacity to provide a communicative environment is certainly influenced by her mental well being and the attachment she has with her child. However, much of this research needs further confirmation in other cohorts. Of particular interest, is whether mothers perceive they are supported in maintaining or increasing their capacity to engage in and provide an adequate communication environment. To date, there is very little research which explores these perceptions.
2.5 The Developmental Stages of Children
2.5.1  Cognitive Development
Cognitive development refers to the progressive and continuous growth of perception, memory, imagination, conception, judgment, and reason; it is the intellectual counterpart of one’s biological adaptation to the environment (Nicolosi, Harryman, & Kresheck, 1989). Cognition also involves the mental activities of comprehending information and the processes of acquiring, organizing, remembering, and using knowledge (Owens, 2008). This knowledge is subsequently used for problem solving and generalization to novel situations.
Many theories have been proposed regarding how children learn about their environment and how cognitive development proceeds. One of the significant theorists in this area, Jean Piaget, viewed the child as an active participant in the learning process. He considered that new learning took place as the child interacted with the environment and with other people.
According to Piaget, cognitive development is based primarily on four factors: maturation, physical experience, social interaction, and a general progression toward equilibrium (Piaget, 1954). Piaget’s theories have withstood the test of time, and more recently have been considered in new light with regard to the effort that a child makes in learning language. For example, Bloom and Tinker (2001) have proposed a model for language development that suggests language emerges out of complex developments in cognition, social–emotional development, and motor skills. This model ties in nicely with what Piaget suggested more than 50 years ago—namely, that the child learns by acting on the environment (and with others). In other words, what begins as  sensorimotor  activity gradually transforms into  complex, abstract  thought.
For Piaget, cognitive or intellectual development is the process of restructuring knowledge. The process begins with a cognitive structure, or a particular way of thinking. This way of thinking is based on what the child currently knows or has experienced. As the child encounters a novel experience,  disequilibrium  is created. The child must compensate for this disturbance and solve the conflict between what he currently knows and his new experience. Piaget referred to this process as adaptation (Piaget, 1954). All organisms must adapt in response to changes in the environment, and it is through this process of adaptation that a child integrates new information.
Two aspects of the adaptation process are key: assimilation and accommodation.
Assimilation refers to the child’s attempts to incorporate new stimuli into existing cognitive schemas (structures). For example, suppose a child is familiar with dogs because there is a German shepherd at home, and then she encounters a poodle. The child is able to assimilate the features of the poodle into the cognitive schema she has for  dogs  because the poodle and the German shepherd share a number of core features. Thus, as the child encounters varied breeds of dogs in her life, she recognizes the common aspects that these animals share and incorporates the new features into her cognitive schema for  dogs . This strategy allows the child to integrate and organize new information and assists in development of categories. When a child encounters a new stimulus that does not fit into his existing cognitive
schema, his cognitive processes undergo the process of  accommodation , whereby new schemes are developed to integrate the new information. Thus, if a child encounters a donkey for the first time, he may initially try to incorporate this new animal into his cognitive schema for dogs . Of course, the donkey is a vastly different type of animal and cannot be integrated into the dog  schema, so a new schema is created. As each of these adaptations in cognition is made, the child is continuing on his path to maintain equilibrium with the environment.
Multiple theories have been put forth regarding the process of cognitive development and the relationships between cognition and language. It is clear that cognition, at least initially, precedes language development, and many cognitive aspects are prerequisites to language development. The relationship between cognition and language likely grows more interdependent over time, as growth in one area fuels growth in another area. Although Piaget’s theory of the stages of cognitive development in young children did not answer all questions about cognition in early childhood, his descriptions of cognitive achievements in childhood can help us understand other aspects of the child’s behavior, including the acquisition of milestones in language development.
In observing children, Piaget noticed patterns in their responses to intellectual tasks.
Children of similar ages responded in ways that were, at the same time, remarkably similar and yet remarkably different from adult responses and expectations. Likewise, children at different ages had their own characteristic way of responding (Labinowicz, 1980).
The first two stages of cognitive development, the sensorimotor and preoperational periods, are collectively termed the  preparatory, prelogical stages . Likewise, the concrete and formal operational stages are collectively termed the  advanced, logical thinking  stages. Each individual stage, however, is characterized by specific developmental milestones. Beginning at birth and extending to age two years, the child coordinates his physical actions. This stage is termed the sensorimotor period. The child’s behavior, while primarily motoric, is described as preoperational and  preverbal;  there is no conceptual thought, although
2.5.2 Motor Development
When we talk about motor development, we usually consider gross motor and fine motor   skills. Gross motor  skills refers to movements involving large muscles, such as trunk muscles used for sitting upright and leg muscles used for walking. Smaller muscles, such as those in the fingers or tongue, are used for  fine motor  tasks, such as writing or talking, respectively. The professionals who are most often involved in the assessment of motor skills in children include physical and occupational therapists. The major motor development achievements of the first 18 months of life. For example, the skill of walking can be expected to emerge anytime between 8 months of age and 18 months of age, with the median age expected to be 12 months. That is a wide window during which this skill may emerge. Thus a child who is not walking at 14 months is not necessarily impaired in gross motor development, although she should be evaluated carefully by an appropriately trained specialist who could assess evidence of other skills involving gross motor development. As can be seen from Figure 2-1, some gross motor skills should emerge earlier in development, such as crawling, walking with assistance, and then standing alone. If these skills are not yet present in a 14-month-old child, there may be greater cause for concern.
2.5.3 Social Emotional Development
Social–emotional development in children is most often evaluated by psychologists and teachers. A child must be interested in socializing and communicating with others to be an effective communicator. Consequently, difficulties with social interaction can profoundly impair communication; indeed, this problem is one of the hallmark features of autism (Gerber, 2003).
In early infancy, we are most concerned with a child’s connectedness with his world. This connectedness is initially expressed through nonverbal modes, such as eye contact and facial expression. The infant will produce a behavior that elicits a reaction in the environment. The infant will respond to this reaction, thereby resulting in learning. For example, in the first month of life, a baby will smile reflexively. There is no evidence that the smile is of itself a planned action. In response, adults in the environment will usually react in a positive manner, using exaggerated vocalizations and laughter. Over time, the infant learns that the smile elicits a very positive reaction from others. Thus, between two and seven months of age, we see the emergence of a social smile, which is a purposeful act. Its development is evidence that learning has taken place for this young child.
A key behavior that is of interest to those who work with young children is the ability to achieve and then maintain self-regulation. Development of regulatory capacity evolves from control of physiological responses to control of emotional state and attention (National Research Council & Institute of Medicine, 2000)—in other words, the child’s ability to maintain homeostasis , or to keep everything in balance. When infants are first born, they have little regulatory capacity beyond keeping their physiological responses in balance, such as body temperature, heart rate, and respiration. These behaviors are not under the baby’s volitional control. Initially, responses to stimulation in the environment are not under the baby’s control, but rather occur automatically, leading them to be called  reflexive  responses. For example, an infant will startle in response to a noise or will begin to suck in response to a touch on the cheek. As the infant matures, these reflexes are integrated as the infant’s ability to self-regulate (i.e., control the body) grows over time. The ability to self-regulate is very helpful to the learning process and is related to the adaptation process described by Piaget’s theory of cognitive development.
All humans are bombarded with sensory input almost constantly. Our bodies are exposed to visual, auditory, tactile, and olfactory stimuli, as well as changes in temperature, motion, and balance. We must make decisions about how and when to respond to each of these stimuli.
As we mature, we learn to ignore certain stimuli and to pay attention to others. This ability to discriminate between stimuli, and then determine which to ignore (or suppress) and which to attend to, is at the core of regulatory development.
Sleep–wake cycles are good examples of the development of regulation. In the first month of life, infants sleep many hours per day. They awaken briefly to feed and then fall asleep again for just a few hours. This pattern continues throughout the day and night, and results in parents who are constantly tired. After the first few weeks of life, the infant is able to maintain the awake state for a longer period of time, which provides the baby with an opportunity to observe and learn from her environment. The time during which the infant remains awake lengthens.
Although the sleep periods lengthen, they decrease in number. This is an example of the development of regulation for the sleep–wake cycle. The result is a rested, alert baby and happy,rested parents! Now consider what happens when a baby is not well or has symptoms of “colic” (where the baby cries for hours at a time): Both baby and adult become dysregulated in this scenario and, as a result, are less attentive to and appreciative of stimuli.
Infants have a preference for faces, and it is through this preference that they establish the foundation for early social relationships with others. Children establish imitation skills by studying others in their environment. This practice begins as imitation of facial expressions (such as smiles) and then develops into imitation of more complex behaviors, such as actions on objects (e.g., throwing a ball, emptying a container), specific motor patterns (e.g., clapping, dancing), and eventually speech and language development. A strong social–emotional foundation is key to the development of verbal language behaviour.
2.5.3 Linguistic Development
In the first two months of life, infants make sounds for limited purposes. They may cry to seek assistance, usually for fulfillment of a physical need such as hunger or other discomfort. When they are calm and in a regulated state, they may make pleasure sounds; these noises may sound like vowels, but are not yet true speech sounds. Nevertheless, these  quasi-resonant nuclei  may form the basis for later sound-making.
As the infant approaches three to four months of age, he produces cooing sounds, which approximate a single syllable consisting of a consonant and a vowel. The child’s production of different vowels also increases and diversifies. Sometime after four months of age, the infant begins to babble, which consists of production of strings of consonant–vowel productions.
Initially these vocalizations will consist of repetitions of the same sound pattern over and over (e.g.,  ba-ba-ba ). As time goes on, the variety of sounds used in one string will vary, and the babble becomes more complex (e.g.,  ba-ta-ba-taba-ti-ba-ti ). Intonation, volume, and pitch begin to vary, too. As the infant approaches 9 to 10 months of age, this vocal pattern is called jargon. At this time, adults will report that the baby seems to be speaking a true language, except without true words. The jargon is often accompanied by gestures and body movements, as well as changes in facial expression. All of these developmental behaviors prepare the child to eventually use true words, first by themselves, and then in strings of longer and longer phrases. Sometime around 10 to 12 months of age, the first word emerges. The first word is typically an approximation of the true word, in that the child must produce a simplified version.
Another development that will occur typically before the first birthday is imitation of adult speech. This seems to be both a word-learning strategy and a social bonding strategy for the child. Imitation serves many purposes for a young child learning to communicate. For instance, it is a way for the child to experiment with words, as adults will provide feedback to the child based on how appropriate the child’s word choice was. It is also a way for the child to engage in a “conversation” with the adult, without requiring the child to have a large expressive vocabulary. Consider the following exchange between a 15-month-old child and her parent while walking in the park:
Parent: Oh, look at the bird! (points to a bird pecking at the ground)
Child: Ba. (gestures toward the bird)
Parent: Oh, do you like the bird? Isn’t it pretty? I think it’s eating. Look at it eat!
Child: A. (while looking at the bird)
Parent: That’s right, it’s eating.
Parent: Oh, it’s going bye-bye. (points to the bird and waves as it flies away)
Child: Ba. (waves)
This brief exchange is a rich example of conversation as a word-learning experience and social opportunity for a young child. The child participates in an event (watching a bird) while hearing a noun ( bird ), a descriptor ( pretty ), and action words ( eat, eating, going, bye-bye ). The simultaneous exposure to an event linked with words provides an opportunity for the child to learn the meanings of words, thereby building both comprehension and vocabulary. Through use of imitation, the child is also able to participate in a verbal exchange with the parent or caregiver, over several turns, while using very few sounds or words.
Bloom and Tinker (2001) have proposed a three-component model of language development, termed the  intentionality model , which emphasizes the impact of  engagement  (social and emotional development) and  effort  (cognitive development) in the process of language acquisition. In this model, the child’s responsiveness to the environment, and his social connectedness to others, establishes for the child what is relevant to learn and what the motivation to learn is. The concept of cognitive effort concerns the work that is required in actively learning about the world. A child must be socially connected to the world and motivated to learn more about the world to figure out how to communicate with others. Thus the intentionality model brings together the components of cognition and social–emotional development and unites them with language in a clear way

Post a Comment

post

     
    Top